Become a member

Language Magazine is a monthly print and online publication that provides cutting-edge information for language learners, educators, and professionals around the world.

― Advertisement ―

― Advertisement ―

Background Knowledge and Where to Get It

Stephen Krashen argues that those who read more know more Wexler (2023) has pointed out that a reader’s background knowledge is a significant predictor...
HomeFeaturesThe Conduit Hypothesis

The Conduit Hypothesis


Stephen Krashen examines how reading leads to academic language competence

 

The field of language education today is dominated by concerns about the development of academic language proficiency, the mastery of the vocabulary, grammar, and discourse style of language needed for complex and specialized functions.

The usual approach is to teach these components directly. I argue here that this approach not only is incorrect but presents students with an impossible task, and that there is a far better path: reading. I describe this path after presenting a few preliminary concepts.

Preliminaries

The central hypothesis underlying the claim of a better path is the comprehension hypothesis, the idea that we acquire language in only one way: when we understand messages or obtain “comprehensible input.” The reading hypothesis is a special case of the comprehension hypothesis and claims that reading is a form of comprehensible input and results in the acquisition of literacy-related aspects of language.

Over the last four decades, I have reported on the substantial and increasing amount of support for the comprehension hypothesis and the reading hypothesis (e.g., Krashen, 1994; 2003; 2004; 2013).

Compelling CI

Language acquirers are obviously more likely to pay attention to input when it is interesting. The compelling comprehensible input hypothesis states that the most effective input is highly interesting, or “compelling”—so interesting that the acquirer is often not even aware that it is another language (Lao and Krashen, 2014).

Self-selected voluntary reading is often compelling, and studies confirm that it is the primary source of our reading ability, ability to write with an acceptable writing style, vocabulary, spelling, and ability to understand and use complex grammatical structures (some evidence provided below).

It has also been established that more self-selected reading leads to more knowledge in a variety of areas, including history, science, and practical matters (Stanovich, West, and Harrison, 1995).

The Conduit Hypothesis

The conduit hypothesis claims that there are three stages in the development of academic language and each stage serves as a conduit for the next, providing the literacy competence as well as the knowledge needed to progress to the next stage.

The hypothesis applies to first- and second-language acquisition. Also, progress can take place without including stage one or two, but it is more difficult.

Stage One: Stories

Read-alouds and hearing stories contribute to language proficiency in two ways. First, they provide the linguistic competence that makes reading written texts more comprehensible.
This includes vocabulary, grammar, and knowledge of how texts are constructed (text structure). This competence is absorbed as the story is heard—it is not an object of study (Mason and Krashen, 2004; Mason et al., 2009; Krashen, 2013).

Second, stories and real-alouds stimulate an interest in books. The title of Brassell’s study tells it all: “Sixteen Books Went Home Tonight: Fifteen Were Introduced by the Teacher” (Brassell, 2003).

Studies also confirm that nearly all children love to hear stories (research reviewed in Krashen, 2004).

Stage Two: Self-Selected Recreational Reading

Stage two consists of massive, but not necessarily wide, self-selected voluntary reading. The reading done in this stage provides the competence and knowledge that makes academic reading more comprehensible.

The reading done in stage two, typically of fiction, forms a bridge between “conversational language” and “academic language.” This idea is confirmed by data from Biber (1988), who analyzed texts in terms of linguistic complexity and reported that fiction fell about midway between conversation and academic texts (abstracts of technical journal papers).

The value of fiction was confirmed by Sullivan and Brown (2014), who found that the amount of reading done was a clear predictor of vocabulary test scores among adult native speakers of English in the UK, controlling for reading done earlier in life. They also reported that the reported frequency of reading high-quality fiction was a very strong predictor of vocabulary knowledge, and reading “middle-brow” fiction was also a good predictor—slightly stronger, in fact, than reading nonfiction. In addition, most of the reading done in the studies cited below showing the value of self-selected voluntary reading was fiction.
Research supporting self-selected reading includes studies of sustained silent reading and correlational/multivariate research.

Sustained Silent Reading (SSR)

Students in classes that include time set aside for voluntary reading in the form of sustained silent reading do better than those in similar classes without sustained silent reading on tests of reading comprehension, vocabulary, writing, and grammar. This is true of first- and second-language studies and holds for children, teenagers, and university students (Krashen, 2004; 2007; Krashen and Mason, 2017).

Correlational and Multivariate Analyses

Here are two examples of this kind of research.

S. Y. Lee (2005) used structural equation modeling, a statistical procedure that allows the investigator to examine complex relationships among variables. Lee reported that the amount of free voluntary reading in English reported was a significant predictor of English writing performance for university students in Taiwan, and the amount of free writing reported was not, clear evidence in favor of the comprehension hypothesis: the amount of input, not output, was related to competence.

Mason and Krashen (2017) did a statistical analysis of the gains made by a group of intermediate adult acquirers of English as a foreign language in Japan who were engaged in self-selected voluntary reading. The subjects gained an average of .6 points on the TOEIC examination for every hour read, and there was little variation among the readers, even though they chose different books to read (largely fiction). This result suggests that a low-intermediate acquirer of English can move to the advanced level in three years of pleasure reading, without study.

Narrow Reading

Readers in stage two and, we shall see, stage three are generally “narrow readers.” Narrow reading is the practice of reading texts by one author or about a single topic of interest, which helps ensure comprehension and natural repetition of vocabulary and grammar (Krashen, 2004).

Evidence of the value of narrow reading includes Lamme (1976), who found that good readers in English as a first language tended to read more books by a single author and books from a series. The evidence also includes Cho and Krashen (1994, 1995a,b), who reported considerable enthusiasm for reading and substantial vocabulary development among adult second-language acquirers who read books in the Sweet Valley series; readers rapidly moved from Sweet Valley Kids (second-grade level) to Sweet Valley Twins (fourth-grade level) to Sweet Valley High (fifth- and sixth-grade level). Several readers in these studies had never read a book in English for pleasure before but became fanatic Sweet Valley fans.

I suspect that many of those who have been successful in using self-selected reading to reach the point where academic texts were comprehensible have been narrow readers.

Stage Three: Narrow Academic Reading

The reading done in stage two, self-selected recreational reading, does not fully provide academic linguistic competence. My claim is that it provides the linguistic and knowledge background that helps make academic reading more comprehensible. The rest of academic competence, I hypothesize, comes from doing a great deal of narrow reading of academic texts in an area of great personal interest to the reader.

As was the case in stage two, reading in stage three is narrow: Bazerman (1985) reported that the physicists he studied only read professional papers that related to their current projects, scanning and filing the others that appeared in current journals for later, if relevant.

Stage-three reading is also compelling, just as exciting for the reader as the fiction of stage two. In my own case, I found that my first adventure in reading linguistic theory, Chomsky’s Syntactic Structures, was as compelling and exciting as reading sports novels and science fiction when I was a teenager.

There is evidence supporting the hypothesis that most of academic linguistic competence must come from reading and not from other sources. It is unlikely that much of academic language competence comes from attending class: in his analysis of text complexity, Biber (2006) reports that classroom discourse is closer to conversational language than to academic language.

Nor does it come from writing. Writing is output, not input, and studies show that more writing does not result in improved writing ability (Krashen, 1994). Even if correction were effective (for evidence that it is not, see Truscott, 2007), we do not write enough or get corrected enough for writing to make a noticeable impact (Krashen, 1994). (1)
Also, gaining academic linguistic proficiency is not the result of studying ”language for academic purposes.”

Can Academic Language Proficiency Be “Learned”?

Current approaches to developing academic language proficiency assume that it must be taught and studied. Scholars describe academic language, these descriptions are presented to students in textbooks and other teaching materials, and students are expected to consciously learn them.

This approach cannot be correct. Most obvious, the system to be mastered is very complex. Scholars, in fact, cannot even agree on the details of the structure of academic writing. Second, there is no clear evidence that anybody has ever mastered more than small bits of pieces of academic language via study. (2)

Acquisition without Learning

I propose that all instances of successful acquisition of academic language are cases of subconscious acquisition, largely as a result of reading. I doubt that any member of the human race has ever consciously learned more than modest amounts of academic language through the study of language for academic purposes.

Notes

Writing, however, can have a profound effect on cognitive development and problem solving: writing can make you smarter. As Elbow has noted, as you move from draft to draft, you come up with new ideas: “Meaning is what you end up with, not what you start out with” (Elbow, 1973, p. 12).

Research on the structure of texts provides clear evidence that text structure cannot be taught directly. The structure of texts is bewilderingly complex. It is hard to imagine any student mastering this knowledge consciously. For example, Swales (1990) presents a “three move” description of introductions to research papers: First the writer “establishes a territory” by “claiming centrality” and/or “making generalizations” and/or reviewing previous research. Then the writer “establishes a niche” by doing one or more of the following: “counter-claiming,” “indicating a gap,” “question-raising,” and/or “continuing a tradition.” Step three is to “occupy the niche” that was created in step two, which is done by “outlining purposes” or “announcing present research” and then announcing the principal findings and indicating the structure of the article.

Those few people who have read a large number of research papers recognize that this description fits. In other words, it provides information that corresponds to what these readers have already subconsciously acquired. The question is whether this information is of use to beginning writers who have not read many or any journal papers.

Swales points out that this analysis, published in 1990, is a revised version of a “four move” description published in 1981. Clearly, the structure of introductions to research articles is a developing area of research where many issues are not settled. This means that it is likely that we are providing students with incomplete and perhaps inaccurate descriptions. And it also means that many people have managed to acquire the structure of research articles without the benefit of accurate descriptions.

The three-move model rapidly becomes more complex as Swales moves into the details. Here is only one example (his chapter on research articles runs 66 pages, with 29 pages devoted just to introductions):

In his discussion of move three, where the writer “occupies the niche” that was created in move two, Swales introduces the “easily applicable” (p. 148) distinction between “integral” and “nonintegral” forms of citation of previous research. At first, this looks simple: Integral citations put the name of the researcher in the sentence, as in (Swales’ example) “Brie (1988) showed that the moon is made of green cheese.” In nonintegral citations, the researcher’s name does not appear in the actual sentence, e.g., “Previous research has shown that the moon is made of cheese (Brie, 1988).” The description then becomes very complicated. Here is just enough to give you an idea: Integral citations include the name of the researcher in several ways, such as a passive agent (“The moon’s cheesy composition was established by Brie (1988)”), as part of a possessive noun phrase (“Brie’s theory (1988) claims that…”), and as an “adjunct of reporting” (“According to Brie (1988), the moon is made of cheese”).

Nonintegral citations are also done in several different ways. Typically they are sentence final—“It has been shown that the moon is made of green cheese (Brie, 1988)”—but there are exceptions, and sometimes writers of research papers do not cite the names of scholars but make reference to schools of thought or name only the most prominent scholar, as in “Chomsky and his coworkers have recently…”, or combinations: “Chomsky and his coworkers (e.g., Napoli, 1982) have recently…”.

And there is more. Research-article citations differ as to whether they use reporting verbs (show, establish, claim) or nonreporting verbs, as in: “According to Brie (1988), the moon is made of green cheese.” Some research shows there is a trend from nonreporting to reporting verbs (Bazerman, 1984, cited in Swales, p. 151; the reader of this paper has undoubtedly noticed (!) that I used a nonintegral citation and a nonreporting verb), but studies also show that most citations are reporting citations (nonintegral, with names of scholars not included, with a reporting verb).

Swales then moves on to a discussion of what verb tenses are used in research articles. There are numerous rules and exceptions. Here is only one example: Swales cites the generalization (Malcolm, 1987) that references to specific experiments tend to be in the past tense. This sounds simple, but it was true in only 61% of the cases in Malcolm’s study. Swales discusses why the rule does not hold in all cases—we might use the present tense if the citation “prepares the way for critical discussion (Malcolm points out that …).”

I have discussed a tiny percentage of Swales’s discussion, but I hope it is enough to give an idea of how hopeless it is to try to teach the structure of academic prose directly. Swales is not clear on the pedagogical implications of his work, sometimes showing some awareness that academic language is acquired from reading rather than consciously learned (p. 90–91) but generally recommending direct teaching. Also, I must emphasize that I have chosen only one example of many. Other scholars have contributed equally complex and confusing descriptions of text structure, recommending that we teach these descriptions to students (e.g., Schleppegrell, Achugar, and Oteiza, 2004).

References

Bazerman, C. (1985). “Physicists Reading physics: Schema-laden purposes and purpose-laden schema.” Written Communication, 2, 3–43.
Biber, D. (1988). Variation across Speech and Writing. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Biber, D. (2006). University Language: A Corpus-Based Study of Spoken and Written Registers. New York: John Benjamins.
Cho, K. S., and Krashen, S. (1994). “Acquisition of Vocabulary from the Sweet Valley High Kids Series: Adult ESL acquisition.” Journal of Reading, 37, 662–667.
Cho, K. S., and Krashen, S. (1995a). “From Sweet Valley Kids to Harlequins in One Year.” California English, 1(1), 18–19.
Cho, K. S., and Krashen, S. (1995b). “Becoming a Dragon: Progress in English as a second language through narrow free voluntary reading.” California Reader, 29, 9–10.
Elbow, P. (1973). Writing without Teachers. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Krashen, S. (1994). “The Input Hypothesis and Its Rivals.” In N. Ellis (ed.), Implicit and Explicit Learning of Languages (45–77). London: Academic Press.
Krashen, S. (2003). Explorations in Language Acquisition and Use: The Taipei Lectures. Portsmouth, NH: Heinemann.
Krashen, S. (2004). The Power of Reading. Portsmouth, NH: Heinemann and Westport: Libraries Unlimited.
Krashen, S. (2003). Explorations in Language Acquisition and Use. Portsmouth, NH: Heinemann.
Krashen, S. (2013). “Read-Alouds: Let’s stick to the story.” Language and Language Teaching, 3 (Azim Premji University and the Vidya Bhawan Society).
Krashen, S. and Mason, B. (2017). “Sustained Silent Reading in Foreign Language Education: An update.” Turkish Online Journal of English Language Teaching, 2, 70–73.
Lao, C. and Krashen, S. (2014). “Language Acquisition without Speaking and without Study.” Journal of Bilingual Education Research and Instruction, 16(1), 215–221.
Lamme, L. (1976). “Are Reading Habits and Abilities Related?” Reading Teacher, 30, 21–27.
Lee, S. Y. (2005). “Facilitating and Inhibiting Factors on EFL Writing: A model testing with SEM.” Language Learning, 55(2), 335–374.
Mason, B., and Krashen, S. (2004). “Is Form-Focused Vocabulary Instruction Worthwhile?” RELC Journal, 35(2), 179–185.
Mason, B., Vanata, M., Jander, K., Borsch, R., and Krashen, S. (2009). “The Effects
and Efficiency of Hearing Stories on Vocabulary Acquisition by Students of German as a
Second Foreign Language in Japan.” Indonesian Journal of English Language
Teaching, 5(1), 1–14.
Schleppegrell, M., Achugar, M., and Oteiza, T. (2004). “The Grammar of History: Enhancing content-based instruction through a functional focus on language.” TESOL Quarterly, 38(1), 67–93.
Swales, J. (1990). Genre Analysis: English in Academic and Research Settings. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Stanovich, K., West, R., and Harrison, M. (1995). “Knowledge Growth and Maintenance across the Life Span: The role of print exposure.” Developmental Psychology, 31(5), 811–826.
Truscott, J. (2007). “The Effect of Error Correction on Learners’ Ability to Write Accurately.” Journal of Second Language Writing, 16, 255–272.

Dr. Stephen Krashen, professor emeritus, Rossier School of Education at USC (University of Southern California), has published more than 300 papers as a linguistic researcher. He is most famously known for his theory of SLA, which includes five hypotheses: the acquisition-learning hypothesis, the input hypothesis, monitor theory, the affective filter, and the natural order hypothesis.

Language Magazine
Send this to a friend